WEKO3
アイテム
幼児はいかに物語を創るか?
http://hdl.handle.net/10083/31410
http://hdl.handle.net/10083/31410974723d1-4483-4946-bdd4-3ac67c7cd882
名前 / ファイル | ライセンス | アクション |
---|---|---|
教育心理学研究30(3)内田.pdf (1.2 MB)
|
|
Item type | 学術雑誌論文 / Journal Article(1) | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
公開日 | 2008-09-29 | |||||
タイトル | ||||||
タイトル | 幼児はいかに物語を創るか? | |||||
タイトル | ||||||
タイトル | HOW DO YOUNG CHILDREN PRODUCE STORIES? | |||||
言語 | en | |||||
言語 | ||||||
言語 | jpn | |||||
資源タイプ | ||||||
資源タイプ識別子 | http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_6501 | |||||
資源タイプ | journal article | |||||
著者 |
内田, 伸子
× 内田, 伸子× Uchida, Nobuko |
|||||
抄録 | ||||||
内容記述タイプ | Abstract | |||||
内容記述 | The purpose of this research was to examine young children's psychological prosess during production stories. At first, two goals were studied in experiment I:the first goal was to find out what effects a story from the daily life or a fancy story, might have on the performance of story production ; the second goal was to clarify developmental changes in children's story production. Eighty 4-year-old and 5-year-old children were divided into 2 homogeneous groups(20 Ss each)at each age level, and assigned to one of two conditions : i.e.a daily life story or a fancy story. Each child heard twice a story providing the information of settings and protagonist's goals, and was asked to complete the story. In order to collect adults' normative protocols, 185 college students were assigned to one of the two conditions randomly. Each story protocol was analyzed in terms of(1)the number of idea units, (2)the coherent connections between episodic chunks, and(3)the story structure according to the narrative categories(exposition, complication, or resolution)developed by Kintsch(1977). The main findings were as follows:Fir\ st, in terms of consistence and coherence of the story structure, the performance of 4-year-old children were much poorer than 5-year-old children under a fancy story condition. Thus, the facility of production of stories seemed to depend on the function of available world and procedural knowledge acquired through experiences. Second, adults' protocols of daily life stories were put into five types of story structures, and the fancy stories were divided into eight types. All types were identified in children's protocolos and closely matched the adults' data. But there were significant differences between children and adults, i.e.adults often produced complete structures made of more complex and elaborated episodes than children. These suggest that first, children have some foundamental logical structures from early childhood ; second, the facility with which individuals can choose and use their knowledge on the world and its process may increase the progress. Next, the two points were investigated in experiment II; the first point was to clarify how children organize stories, i.e.what strategies they use in story construction ; the second point was to confirm the results of the experiment I. One hundred and twenty 4-year-old\ and 5-year-old children were divided into 3 homogeneous groups(20 Ss each)at each age level, and assigned to one of three conditions ; H gr.(was presented ten sentences with a fancy story beginning and a happy ending sentence), U gr.(was presented the ten sentences and an unhappy ending sentence), and C gr.(was presented the ten sentences only). The task of H and U conditions was to complete the story by connecting the beginning and the end, and that of C condition was to be the same as experiment I. The main results were as follows: First, 5-year-old children could complete coherent stories in any conditions, but 4-year-old children could not generate coherent stories under H and U conditions. This result was interpreted in terms of the development of planning competence in story construction, i.e.this suggests that 5-year-old children can use either global plot plan strategies or local plan(what-next)strategies, but 4-year-old cannot use plot plan strategies. Second, exclusive of uncodable protocols of 4-year-old, and other protocols were identified with eight types of story structures. This confirmed the second finding of experiment I. |
|||||
書誌情報 |
教育心理学研究 巻 30, 号 3, p. 211-222, 発行日 1982-09-30 |
|||||
ISSN | ||||||
収録物識別子タイプ | ISSN | |||||
収録物識別子 | 00215015 | |||||
書誌レコードID | ||||||
収録物識別子タイプ | NCID | |||||
収録物識別子 | AN00345837 | |||||
権利 | ||||||
権利情報 | 日本教育心理学会 | |||||
権利 | ||||||
権利情報 | 本文データは学協会の許諾に基づきCiNiiから複製したものである | |||||
フォーマット | ||||||
内容記述タイプ | Other | |||||
内容記述 | application/pdf | |||||
形態 | ||||||
値 | 1233157 bytes | |||||
著者版フラグ | ||||||
値 | publisher | |||||
日本十進分類法 | ||||||
主題Scheme | NDC | |||||
主題 | 370 | |||||
NIIサブジェクト | ||||||
主題Scheme | Other | |||||
主題 | 教育・教育学 | |||||
出版者 | ||||||
出版者 | 日本教育心理学会 | |||||
資源タイプ | ||||||
内容記述タイプ | Other | |||||
内容記述 | 学術雑誌論文 | |||||
資源タイプ・ローカル | ||||||
値 | 学術雑誌論文 | |||||
資源タイプ・NII | ||||||
値 | Journal Article | |||||
資源タイプ・DCMI | ||||||
値 | text | |||||
資源タイプ・ローカル表示コード | ||||||
値 | 01 | |||||
異版である | ||||||
関連タイプ | isVersionOf | |||||
識別子タイプ | URI | |||||
関連識別子 | http://ci.nii.ac.jp/naid/110001892435/ | |||||
所属 | ||||||
値 | お茶の水女子大学文教育学部 |